Think Your Opinion Must Be Right? Science Reveals Why You May Be Wrong

Medically reviewed by Drugs.com.

By Dennis Thompson HealthDay Reporter

THURSDAY, Oct. 10, 2024 -- Attention all 'know-it-alls."

Folks who are sure they're right often believe they've got enough information to make up their minds, even if in reality they only have part of the picture, a new study finds.

It’s a concept called the “illusion of information adequacy,” and it helps explain how people can have such strong and cemented opinions even though they get their news from limited and biased sources, researchers said.

“We found that, in general, people don’t stop to think whether there might be more information that would help them make a more informed decision,” said researcher Angus Fletcher, a professor of English at Ohio State University.

“If you give people a few pieces of information that seems to line up, most will say ‘that sounds about right’ and go with that,” Fletcher said in an Ohio State news release.

For the study, researchers recruited nearly 1,300 Americans for an online experiment.

All of the people read an article about a fictional school that lacked adequate water.

But one group were provided an article containing only reasons why the school should merge with another that had adequate water. A second group’s article only gave reasons for staying separate and hoping for other solutions.

Only a third group, which served as the control group, had an article that provided all the arguments for either merging or staying separate.

The two groups that read only half the story still believed they had enough information to make a solid decision, and said they would follow the recommendations in the article provided to them, results show.

“Those with only half the information were actually more confident in their decision to merge or remain separate than those who had the complete story,” Fletcher said. “They were quite sure that their decision was the right one, even though they didn’t have all the information.”

Folks with half the information also thought most other people would come to the same decision as them, researchers added.

However, the experiment offered one ray of hope.

People who had read only one side of the story were later provided arguments for the other side, and many were willing to change their minds once they had all the facts, researchers said.

The new study was published Oct. 9 in the journal PLOS One.

That might not work all the time, particularly on issues related to entrenched ideological positions, Fletcher said.

In those cases, people might reject the new information as untrustworthy or try to reframe it to fit in with their preexisting views.

“But most interpersonal conflicts aren’t about ideology,” Fletcher said. “They are just misunderstandings in the course of daily life.”

People need to make sure they have the full story about a situation before they take a stand or make a decision, rather than leading with their chin, Fletcher said.

“As we found in this study, there’s this default mode in which people think they know all the relevant facts, even if they don’t,” Fletcher said.

“Your first move when you disagree with someone should be to think, ‘Is there something that I’m missing that would help me see their perspective and understand their position better?’” Fletcher continued. “That’s the way to fight this illusion of information adequacy.”

Sources

  • Ohio State University, news release, Oct. 9, 2024
  • Disclaimer: Statistical data in medical articles provide general trends and do not pertain to individuals. Individual factors can vary greatly. Always seek personalized medical advice for individual healthcare decisions.

    Source: HealthDay

    Read more

    Disclaimer

    Every effort has been made to ensure that the information provided by Drugslib.com is accurate, up-to-date, and complete, but no guarantee is made to that effect. Drug information contained herein may be time sensitive. Drugslib.com information has been compiled for use by healthcare practitioners and consumers in the United States and therefore Drugslib.com does not warrant that uses outside of the United States are appropriate, unless specifically indicated otherwise. Drugslib.com's drug information does not endorse drugs, diagnose patients or recommend therapy. Drugslib.com's drug information is an informational resource designed to assist licensed healthcare practitioners in caring for their patients and/or to serve consumers viewing this service as a supplement to, and not a substitute for, the expertise, skill, knowledge and judgment of healthcare practitioners.

    The absence of a warning for a given drug or drug combination in no way should be construed to indicate that the drug or drug combination is safe, effective or appropriate for any given patient. Drugslib.com does not assume any responsibility for any aspect of healthcare administered with the aid of information Drugslib.com provides. The information contained herein is not intended to cover all possible uses, directions, precautions, warnings, drug interactions, allergic reactions, or adverse effects. If you have questions about the drugs you are taking, check with your doctor, nurse or pharmacist.

    Popular Keywords